Which of the following is documented, would support the recalibration theory

Sensorimotor changes are well documented following reaches with altered visual feedback of the hand. Specifically, reaches are adapted and proprioceptive estimates of felt hand position shifted in the direction of the visual feedback experienced. While research has examined one's ability to retain reach adaptation, limited attention has been given to the retention of proprioceptive recalibration. This experiment examined retention of proprioceptive recalibration in the form of recall and savings (i.e., faster proprioceptive recalibration on subsequent testing days) over an extended period of time (i.e., four days). As well, we looked to determine the benefits of additional training on short-term retention (i.e., one day) of proprioceptive recalibration. Twenty-four participants trained to reach to a visual target while seeing a cursor that was rotated 30° clockwise relative to their hand on an initial day of testing. Half of the participants then completed additional reach training trials on 4 subsequent testing days (Training group), whereas the second half of participants did not complete additional training until Day 5 (Non-Training group). Participants provided estimates of their felt hand position on all 5 testing days to establish retention of proprioceptive recalibration. Results revealed that proprioceptive recalibration was recalled 24 h after initial training across all participants. Recall of proprioceptive recalibration was not observed on subsequent testing days for the Non-Training group, while recall of proprioceptive recalibration was retained at a similar level across all subsequent testing days for the Training group. Retention of proprioceptive recalibration in the form of savings was observed on Day 5 in the Non-Training group. These results reveal that short-term recall of proprioceptive recalibration does not benefit from additional training. Moreover, the different time scales (i.e., retention in the form of recall seen only at 24 h after initial training versus savings observed 4 days after initial training in the Non-Training group), suggest that distinct processes may underlie recall and savings of proprioceptive recalibration.

Show

Introduction

When reaching to visual objects in one's environment, a series of sensory to motor transformations occurs in order to ensure that one successfully reaches to a desired object. Specifically, visual information related to the hand and target position and proprioceptive information regarding hand position are transformed into appropriate motor commands (Desmurget et al., 1998, Flanders et al., 1992, Jeannerod, 1988). In cases when movements are performed incorrectly (e.g., due to changes within the environment and/or one's body), one tends to rely more on the visual estimate of the limb, rather than the actual or “felt” position to correct their movement. For example, when reaching in a virtual reality environment with distorted visual feedback of the hand, individuals use the distorted visual feedback to rapidly alter their reaches on subsequent trials in order for the visual representation of their hand to achieve the target. This process is referred to as visuomotor adaptation (Cressman and Henriques, 2009, Krakauer et al., 1999, Krakauer et al., 2005, Simani et al., 2007). After adapting their movements in response to the altered visual feedback, individuals continue to reach with the newly acquired movement pattern, even when visual feedback is removed. These reach errors, known as aftereffects, are evidence that persistent motor changes have occurred and an internal model adapted (Baraduc and Wolpert, 2002, Buch et al., 2003, Cressman et al., 2010, Cressman and Henriques, 2009, Cressman and Henriques, 2010, Krakauer et al., 1999, Krakauer et al., 2000, Martin et al., 1996, Zbib et al., 2016).

In addition to changes in reaches, sensory changes have also been shown to occur following reach training with misaligned visual feedback of the hand. Specifically, participants recalibrate their sense of felt hand position (i.e., proprioceptive recalibration), such that after reaching in a virtual reality environment with distorted visual feedback of the hand, they alter their perception of where they feel their hand is to more closely align with the visual feedback provided (Cressman and Henriques, 2009, Zbib et al., 2016). This proprioceptive recalibration has been assessed in tasks during which the participant's hand is guided out passively to different locations in the workspace by a robot manipulandum and the participant indicates the position of their hand relative to a visual or proprioceptive reference marker (Cressman and Henriques, 2009), as well as by performing tasks requiring participants to actively reach to a proprioceptive target (Ruttle et al., 2016, Simani et al., 2007, van Beers et al., 2002).

Recently, Zbib et al. (2016) looked to determine how quickly proprioception was recalibrated when training to reach with a cursor that was rotated 30° clockwise (CW) relative to hand motion. They found that shifts in proprioceptive estimates took longer to arise in comparison to visuomotor adaptation as assessed through aftereffect trials. Specifically, Zbib et al. (2016) found that proprioception had shifted by 8.8° (~30% of the 30° cursor distortion) relative to baseline after participants completed 70 rotated reach training trials, while reaches were adapted by 16.9° (~56% of the 30° distortion) after only 5 reach training trials. Moreover, the shifts in proprioception observed after 70 trials did not increase significantly again with an additional 80 trials of training, such that proprioceptive recalibration plateaued at 8.8° (or 30% of the 30° distortion). On the other hand, the early reach errors observed plateaued after only 40 trials of training at 22.3° (or 74.3% of the 30° distortion). A more recent study by Ruttle and colleagues (2016), also showed greater visuomotor adaptation after only 6 reach training trials with visual feedback rotated relative to hand motion (10.5° or 26.2% of the 40° visuomotor rotation distortion) compared to proprioceptive recalibration following those same 6 training trials (5.2° or 13% of the 40° visuomotor rotation distortion). Like Zbib et al. (2016), proprioceptive recalibration did not continue to significantly increase over additional reach training trials, such that at the end of reach training, the extent of proprioceptive recalibration was much less than visuomotor adaptation. Interestingly, additional reach training sessions within a single testing day do not appear to lead to greater proprioceptive recalibration (Salomonczyk et al., 2011). Together, these results suggest that proprioceptive recalibration arises more slowly compared to visuomotor adaptation and that proprioception may only be able to be recalibrated up to a maximal point, well below the magnitude of visuomotor adaptation.

While the time course and magnitude of changes with respect to reach aftereffects and proprioceptive recalibration following training with distorted visual feedback of the hand have been shown to differ in an initial testing session, both show evidence of retention. Retention can be established by observing (1) aftereffects at the start of a second testing session, which we will refer to as recall (Mawase et al., 2017) and/or (2) savings within a second testing session, such that there is faster relearning upon exposure to a previously experienced environment compared to initial training performance (Huberdeau et al., 2015, Krakauer, 2009, Zarahn et al., 2008). Retention of visuomotor adaptation has been firmly established by observing the recall of aftereffects and savings at various times following training with the initial visuomotor rotation perturbation (e.g., 24 h (Nourouzpour et al., 2014), 1–2 week(s) and 12 months (Yamamoto et al., 2006) post initial training). For example, Nourouzpour et al. (2014) found retention of visuomotor adaptation following reach training with a 40° CW visuomotor rotation, such that immediate aftereffects were not different from those measured 24 h later. Furthermore, results from Yamamoto et al. (2006) showed significant long-lasting changes in motor performance in the form of recall and suggested that even relatively short-term reach training with distorted visual feedback of the hand (e.g., 120 trials for humans & 150 trials for monkeys) leads to long-term changes in reaches (1–2 week(s) & > 1 year). With respect to savings of visuomotor adaptation, Klassen et al. (2005) observed that reaches were adapted more rapidly in a second testing session 24 h after training with a cursor rotated 30° CCW relative to the hand compared to the initial session. Furthermore, studies have shown similar results in follow-up testing sessions 48 h (Caithness et al., 2004) and 1 week (Krakauer et al., 2005) after initial training. While measures of recall and savings of visuomotor adaptation both provide insight into the long-term retention of motor adaptation, they are suggested to arise due to different underlying processes (e.g., an adapted internal model vs. increased error sensitivity (Herzfeld et al., 2014) and/or explicit processes (Huberdeau et al., 2015), respectively). In accordance with this suggestion, Mawase et al. (2017) have recently shown that the neural circuitry underlying retention in the form of recall and savings of (locomotor) adaptation differ.

To date, studies examining retention of proprioceptive recalibration following reach training are limited. Specifically, studies examining retention of proprioceptive recalibration following visuomotor adaptation trials have focused on examining retention in the form of recall 24 h after initial training (Nourouzpour et al., 2014, Ostry et al., 2010), with no examination into potential savings. Within these previous studies, Ostry et al. (2010) showed that changes in sense of limb motion persisted 24 h after reaching in a velocity dependent force-field, while Nourouzpour et al. (2014) found that changes in felt hand position persisted 24 h after adapting to a gradually introduced visuomotor rotation distortion. Results from Hatada et al. (2006) suggest that longer-term retention of sensory changes (e.g., up to 7 days) may be possible, as they found continued changes in reaches to proprioceptive targets 7 days after participants reached to targets while wearing laterally displacing prism lenses, with the greatest recall occurring two days after participants completed initial training. Furthermore, Berniker and Kording, 2008, Berniker and Kording, 2011 source-estimation model also posits retention of sensory changes. Specifically, their model, which suggests that the source of errors experienced during motor adaptation are assigned to a combination of changes in both world-based or body-based properties, predicts that recalibration of our proprioceptive (body-based) estimates should persist over time in the form of savings.

The goal of the current study was to establish retention of proprioceptive recalibration over an extended period of time (4 days) in the form of recall and savings. As well, we looked to determine the benefits of additional training on short-term (24 h) retention of proprioceptive recalibration. All of our participants trained to reach to a visual target when provided with rotated visual feedback of the hand (i.e., a 30° CW cursor rotation). Over the course of the reach training trials on an initial day of testing, we had participants reach to the target without visual feedback to assess the timeline of visuomotor adaptation in the form of aftereffects and estimate where they felt their hand was located in space to determine the timeline of proprioceptive recalibration. Following this initial day of testing, participants were divided into two groups: one with similar, additional, reach training across 4 consecutive days of testing (Training group) and another which did not perform the additional reach training trials (Non-Training group) on days 2 through 4. On the 5th testing day, all participants performed the reach training trials. We measured retention in the form of recall on 4 consecutive days of testing following the initial training day. As well, savings was assessed on each training day in the Training group and in the Non-Training group on Day 5. Similar to previous results (e.g., Nourouzpour et al., 2014), we hypothesized that all participants would recalibrate their sense of felt hand position on the first day of testing and recall these sensory changes 24 h following initial training. Furthermore, given that proprioceptive recalibration has previously shown to be limited in magnitude compared to reach adaptation (Salomonczyk et al., 2011), we hypothesized that the Training group would exhibit a greater magnitude of proprioceptive recalibration as training progressed, thus potentially leading to greater recall of proprioceptive recalibration across the testing days as well. For the Non-Training group, we expected recall of proprioceptive recalibration to decrease across testing days with increased time from reaching with the misaligned visual feedback and hence experiencing the visual-proprioceptive mismatch related to hand position. However, we hypothesized that savings of proprioceptive recalibration would be evident on Day 5, given findings from motor adaptation literature suggesting that different processes, including neural networks, underlie savings and recall (Mawase et al., 2017).

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-four young adults (9 female, 15 male; mean age = 22.1 years, SD = 2.3) were recruited and took part in this experiment. Participants were divided into two groups: i) a Training group (n = 12) and ii) a Non-Training group (n = 12). All participants were deemed healthy following verbal screening for sensory, neurological or motor dysfunctions. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and prior to beginning the experiment all completed the 10 item version of the Edinburgh

Proprioceptive recalibration

In order to establish if participants shifted their felt hand location during reach training trials and if this changed across days, we first determined the locations that participants felt their hands were aligned with the visual reference marker. This was done by fitting a logistic function to each participant's responses during the proprioceptive estimate trials completed within each block and calculating their bias (i.e., the point at which participants responded left and right 50% of the

Discussion

The goal of the present experiment was to examine retention of proprioceptive recalibration over an extended period of time (4 days) in the form of recall and savings. As well, we looked to determine the benefits of additional training on short-term retention of proprioceptive recalibration. Participants completed reach training trials (i.e., 150 reaches), during which they reached to a visual target when provided with rotated visual feedback of the hand (i.e., a cursor rotated 30° CW relative

Conclusion

In the current study, we found retention of proprioceptive recalibration in the form of recall (24 h post initial training) and savings (4 days post initial training). Moreover, this savings of proprioceptive recalibration observed in the Non-Training group 4 days post initial training was present even when these participants did not display any recall of proprioceptive recalibration. Additional training did not lead to any further benefits in the short-term retention of proprioceptive

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grant awarded to E.K.C (RGPIN 386881-2011).

References (51)

  • R.J. van Beers et al.

    When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation

    Curr. Biol.

    (2002)

  • B. Treutwein

    Adaptive psychophysical procedures

    Vision. Res.

    (1995)

  • D. Salomonczyk et al.

    Proprioceptive recalibration following prolonged training and increasing distortions in visuomotor adaptation

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)

  • G.M. Redding et al.

    Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2005)

  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)

  • M. Desmurget et al.

    From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1998)

  • P. Baraduc et al.

    Adaptation to a visuomotor shift depends on the starting posture

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2002)

  • M. Berniker et al.

    Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2008)

  • M. Berniker et al.

    Estimating the relevance of world disturbances to explain savings, interference and long-term motor adaptation effects

    PLoS Comput. Biol.

    (2011)

  • L.E. Brown et al.

    Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2003)

  • E.R. Buch et al.

    Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging

    Learn. Mem.

    (2003)

  • G. Caithness et al.

    Failure to consolidate the consolidation theory of learning for sensorimotor adaptation tasks

    J. Neurosci.

    (2004)

  • E.K. Cressman et al.

    Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)

  • E.K. Cressman et al.

    Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2009)

  • E.K. Cressman et al.

    Visuomotor adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration in older adults

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2010)

  • A. Crowe et al.

    Proprioceptive accuracy in two dimensions

    Percept. Mot. Skills.

    (1987)

  • M. Flanders et al.

    Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1992)

  • Hadjiosef, A.M., Smith, M.A., 2013a. Savings is restricted to the temporally labile component of motor adaptation....
  • Hadjiosef, A.M., Smith, M.A., 2013b. Generalization of temporally-labile and temporally stable components of motor...
  • Y. Hatada et al.

    Two waves of a long-lasting aftereffect of prism adaptation measured over 7 days

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2006)

  • D.J. Herzfeld et al.

    A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning

    Science

    (2014)

  • D.M. Huberdeau et al.

    Formation of a long-term memory for visuomotor adaptation following only a few practice trials

    J. Neurophsyiol.

    (2015)

  • Jeannerod, M., 1988. The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements, Oxford psychology series, No....
  • S.A.H. Jones et al.

    Proprioceptive localization of the left and right hands

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2010)

  • H. Kesten

    Accelerated stochastic approximation

    Ann. Math. Stat.

    (1958)

  • Navigate DownView more references

    Cited by (8)

    • Mechanisms of proprioceptive realignment in human motor learning

      2021, Current Opinion in Physiology

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Adaptation is a form of motor learning that alters movement in response to a predictable perturbation to the body or environment. This process can also realign or bias proprioception of the adapted movement. Here, we discuss recent work on two mechanisms that are suggested to underlie proprioceptive realignment following motor learning: a sensory mechanism that hinges on mismatches between estimates of limb state across sensory modalities, and a motor mechanism driven by the recalibration of sensory predictions of movement. We review work focused on normal behavior, neuroimaging studies and assessments of neurologic patients suggesting that motor systems, such as the cerebellum, and sensory association systems, such as somatosensory and posterior parietal cortices, may play a role in proprioceptive realignment.

    • Improved proprioception does not benefit visuomotor adaptation

      2022, Experimental Brain Research

    • The Influence of Age on the Intermanual Transfer and Retention of Implicit Visuomotor Adaptation

      2022, Journal of Motor Behavior

    • Intermanual transfer and retention of visuomotor adaptation to a large visuomotor distortion are driven by explicit processes

      2021, PLoS ONE

    • Visual and proprioceptive recalibrations after exposure to a visuomotor rotation

      2019, European Journal of Neuroscience

    • Motor learning without moving: Proprioceptive and predictive hand localization after passive visuoproprioceptive discrepancy training

      2019, PLoS ONE

    Arrow Up and RightView all citing articles on Scopus

    • Research article

      The effect of motor context on semantic processing: A TMS study

      Neuropsychologia, Volume 114, 2018, pp. 243-250

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Strong embodiment theories claimed that action language representation is grounded in the sensorimotor system, which would be crucially to semantic understanding. However, there is a large disagreement in literature about the neural mechanisms involved in abstract (symbolic) language comprehension.

      In the present study, we investigated the role of motor context in the semantic processing of abstract language. We hypothesized that motor cortex excitability during abstract word comprehension could be modulated by previous presentation of a stimuli which associated a congruent motor content (i.e., a semantically related gesture) to the word.

      We administered a semantic priming paradigm where postures of gestures (primes) were followed by semantically congruent verbal stimuli (targets, meaningful or meaningless words). Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation was delivered to left motor cortex 100, 250 and 500 ms after the presentation of each target. Results showed that Motor evoked potentials of hand muscle significantly increased in correspondence to meaningful compared to meaningless words, but only in the earlier phase of semantic processing (100 and 250 ms from target onset).

      Results suggested that the gestural motor representation was integrated with corresponding word meaning in order to accomplish (and facilitate) the lexical task. We concluded that motor context resulted crucial to highlight motor system involvement during semantic processing of abstract language.

    • Research article

      Neuropsychologia, Volume 114, 2018, pp. 50-64

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Studies investigating the strength and membership of regions within multiple functional networks primarily focus on either resting state or single cognitive tasks. The goals of the current study were to investigate whether task-related functional connectivity changes with task complexity, and whether this connectivity-complexity relationship is age-sensitive. We assessed seed-to-voxel functional connectivity for the default mode network (DMN) and two attentional networks [cingulo-opercular (CO), fronto-parietal (FP)] in three cognitive control tasks of increasing complexity (Single task, Dual task, and Memory Updating), across younger and older adults (N = 52; NYoung = 23; NOld = 29). The three tasks systematically varied in cognitive control demands due to differing maintenance, switching, and updating requirements. Functional connectivity for all networks, resulting from task > rest contrasts, increased with greater task complexity, irrespective of age and gray matter volume. Moreover, between-network connectivity for DMN, CO, and FP regions was greatest for working memory updating, the most complex task. Regarding age-related differences in accuracy, none were observed for Single or Dual tasks, but older adults had poorer accuracy in Memory Updating. More anterior frontal clusters of functional connectivity were observed for older, compared to younger, adults; these were limited to seeds of the two attentional networks. Importantly, increased connectivity in these additional frontal regions in older adults were non-compensatory, because they were associated with detrimental task performance, especially Memory Updating. For the Memory Updating > Rest, the younger > older contrast resulted in greater DMN seed connectivity to regions in the other two attentional networks, implicating increased reliance on between-network connectivity for the DMN seeds during complex cognitive tasks. Our results also implicate functional connectivity between attentional networks and the cerebellum during cognitive control. Reliability of multiple seeds in the seed-to-voxel connectivity is also discussed.

    • Research article

      Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition

      Current Biology, Volume 23, Issue 12, 2013, pp. 1107-1112

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Current models of motor learning posit that skill acquisition involves both the formation and decay of multiple motor memories that can be engaged in different contexts [, , , , , , , , ]. Memory formation is assumed to be context dependent, so that errors most strongly update motor memories associated with the current context. In contrast, memory decay is assumed to be context independent, so that movement in any context leads to uniform decay across all contexts. We demonstrate that for both object manipulation and force-field adaptation, contrary to previous models, memory decay is highly context dependent. We show that the decay of memory associated with a given context is greatest for movements made in that context, with more distant contexts showing markedly reduced decay. Thus, both memory formation and decay are strongest for the current context. We propose that this apparently paradoxical organization provides a mechanism for optimizing performance. While memory decay tends to reduce force output [, ], memory formation can correct for any errors that arise, allowing the motor system to regulate force output so as to both minimize errors and avoid unnecessary energy expenditure. The motor commands for any given context thus result from a balance between memory formation and decay, while memories for other contexts are preserved.

    • Research article

      Explicit and implicit components of visuo-motor adaptation: An analysis of individual differences

      Consciousness and Cognition, Volume 33, 2015, pp. 156-169

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Adaptation to visuo-motor rotations embraces implicit and explicit components. We contrast this two-component model with a three-component model by means of an individual-differences approach. Adaptive changes were tested under four conditions: (1) closed-loop test, presence of the rotation cued (initial adaptive shift), (2) open-loop test, presence of the rotation cued (adaptive shift), (3) open-loop test, absence of the rotation cued (after-effect), (4) test of explicit knowledge (explicit shift). After-effects and explicit shifts were uncorrelated. After regression on after-effects and explicit shifts, the residuals of the initial adaptive shifts and the adaptive shifts remained correlated, suggesting an additional implicit component of adaptation found only in the cued presence of the visuo-motor rotation. The two implicit components are consistent with the distinction between a change of the body schema giving rise to after-effects, and the development of an internal model of a tool that is applied only when the transformation is present.

    • Research article

      Frequency characteristics of heteronymous responses evoked by Achilles tendon vibration during quiet stance

      Neuroscience Letters, Volume 736, 2020, Article 135290

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Primary (Ia) sensory afferents that innervate muscle spindles provide strong synaptic input to homonymous motoneurons and are thought to play a role in balance control. In addition, Ia afferents have broad heteronymous connections; i.e., projections to motoneurons that innervate other muscles that act at the same joint as well as at different joints. The purpose of the current study was to investigate heteronymous Ia afferent connections from the triceps surae muscles to lower limb and back muscles during quiet standing in humans. We applied supra-threshold noisy vibration (10−115 Hz) to the right Achilles tendon of 12 participants maintaining quiet stance and recorded EMG activity bilaterally from homonymous (Soleus) and heteronymous muscles (Semitendinosus, Vastus Lateralis, Erector Spinae). We estimated coherence, phase, and gain between the tendon probe acceleration and rectified EMG from each muscle. We found significant coherence between the probe acceleration and EMG in ipsilateral Soleus (5−100 Hz), Semitendinosus (10−75 Hz), Vastus Lateralis (5-70 Hz), and bilateral Erector Spinae muscles (10−70 Hz). These results provide evidence that triceps surae muscle spindle afferents can influence the activity of muscles proximal to the ankle joint across a broad frequency band during quiet standing.

    • Research article

      A rare case of deafferentation reveals an essential role of proprioception in bilateral coordination

      Neuropsychologia, Volume 160, 2021, Article 107969

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Loss of proprioception has been shown to produce deficits in intralimb coordination and in the ability to stabilize limb posture in the absence of visual feedback. However, the role of proprioceptive signals in the feedforward and feedback control of interlimb coordination remains unclear. To address this issue, we examined bimanual coordination in a deafferented participant (DP) with large-fiber sensory neuropathy, which resulted in the loss of proprioception and touch in both arms, and in age-matched control participants. The task required participants to move a single virtual bar with both hands to a rectangular target with horizontal orientation. The participants received visual feedback of the virtual bar, but not of the hand positions along the bar-axis. Although the task required symmetrical movement between the arms, there were significant differences in the trajectories of the dominant and non-dominant hands in the deafferented participant, and thus more final errors and impaired coordination compared to controls. Deafferentation was also associated with an asymmetric deficit in stabilizing the hand at the end of motion, where the dominant arm showed more drift than the non-dominant arm. While the findings with DP may reflect a unique adaptation to deafferentation, they suggest that 1) Bilateral coordination depends on proprioceptive feedback, and 2) Postural stability at the end of motion can be specified through feedforward mechanisms, in the absence of proprioceptive feedback, but this process appears to be asymmetric, with better stability in the non-dominant arm.