What is the utility of a process model of cognition over a verbal description?

Few would argue against the position that spatial cognition involves cognition. Much of spatial cognition research has focused on illuminating the domain-general processes (e.g. attention, memory, or representation) active in this domain-specific field. In this chapter, we suggest that researchers view this domain-general to domain-specific relationship in the opposite direction. In other words, we review spatial cognition research within the context of its utility for understanding domain-general processes. For a cognitive process to be domain-general, it should be evident across a wide variety of domain-specific tasks, including verbal and spatial ones. Yet, the majority of data supporting domain-general models comes from verbal tasks, such as list learning. Thus, we suggest that considering spatial cognition tasks and findings along with those from other domains would enhance our understanding of truly domain-general processing.

Show

Section snippets

Spatial Cognition: Exploring the Domain-general in the Domain-specific

Spatial cognition as a subdiscipline of cognition falls under a category referred to as domain-specific cognition. This category has an isolating effect. It suggests work that is narrowly focused, thus carrying implications for how the larger field of Cognitive Science views spatial cognition research. The outcome of this narrow view is what happens in spatial cognition research stays within spatial cognition research. Spatial cognition citations appear in other spatial cognition studies, but

Spatial Cognition and Attention

Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration, of consciousness are of its essence.

William James (1890; pp. 403–404).

Attention underlies both basic (e.g. perception and memory) and higher-order cognition, including spatial cognition. Without attention, information receives minimal, if any, processing. Two everyday spatial cognition

Memory and Spatial Cognition

Memory research covers a broad range of specific issues, many which have captured prominent research attention. It is probably the cognitive subfield in which the most research has been devoted to developing theoretical positions and computational models. However, the majority of these models rely on data from verbal learning paradigms. The generalizability of these models would be enhanced by considering results from spatial memory research.

Representing Spatial Information: Embodiment

Theories of how humans represent information can be bifurcated into abstract/amodal views (Fodor, 1975; Pylyshyn, 1984) and more recent embodied views (Grafton, 2009; Lakoff, 1988; Wilson, 2002). Embodied views posit that we represent information through visual and motor information available when learning (e.g. Glenberg, 1997; Zwaan, 2004). Then, when recalling information, people evoke perceptual and/or action simulations. Some experimental evidence supports such simulations. When people

The Cognition of Spatial Cognition

In this chapter, we have discussed spatial cognition research relative to three cognitive processes: attention, memory, and representation. Few would argue that understanding domain-general aspects of these processes leads to a stronger theoretical understanding of spatial cognition. To this end, bringing together, in one place, a discussion of how attention, memory, and representation issues bear out in spatial cognition has utility. We argue that discussing the reverse relationship may have

References (189)

  • E.H. Lichtenstein et al.

    Memory for goal-directed events

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1980)

  • S.M. Kosslyn

    Information representation in visual images

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1975)

  • S. Kita et al.

    What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2003)

  • K. Kessler et al.

    Embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference

    Cognition

    (2010)

  • E.A. Kensinger et al.

    Memory for specific visual details can be enhanced by negative arousing content

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2006)

  • D. Kempler et al.

    Sentence comprehension deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: a comparison of off-line vs. on-line sentence processing

    Brain and Language

    (1998)

  • C. Hölscher et al.

    Would you follow your own route description? Cognitive strategies in urban route planning

    Cognition

    (2011)

  • L. Hermer et al.

    Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation

    Cognition

    (1996)

  • J. Hatfield et al.

    The effects of mobile phone use on pedestrian crossing behavior at signalised and unsignalised intersections

    Accident Analysis and Prevention

    (2007)

  • P.W. Halligan et al.

    Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect

    (2003)

  • N.R. Ellis et al.

    Developmental aspects of memory for spatial location

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1987)

  • T. Ditman et al.

    Simulating an enactment effect: pronouns guide action simulation during narrative comprehension

    Cognition

    (2010)

  • G.L. Clore et al.

    How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought

    (2007)

  • M.M. Chun et al.

    Interactions between attention and memory

    Current Opinions in Neurobiology

    (2007)

  • L.A. Carlson-Radvansky et al.

    Reference frame activation during spatial term assignment

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (1994)

  • L.A. Carlson-Radvansky et al.

    Frames of reference in vision and language: where is above?

    Cognition

    (1993)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Working memory in developing and applying mental models from spatial descriptions

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2008)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Moving through imagined space: mentally simulating locomotion during spatial description reading

    Acta Psychologica

    (2010)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Emotional state and local vs. global spatial memory

    Acta Psychologica

    (2009)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    You heard it here first: readers mentally simulate described sounds

    Acta Psychologica

    (2010)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Planning routes around the world: international evidence for southern route preferences

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (2012)

  • G.H. Bower et al.

    Scripts in memory for text

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1979)

  • E. Awh et al.

    Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory

    (2001)

  • W.S. Albert et al.

    The role of attention in spatial learning during simulated route navigation

    Environment and Planning A

    (1999)

  • C. Aporta et al.

    Satellite culture: global positioning systems, inuit wayfinding, and the need for a new account of technology

    Current Anthropology

    (2005)

  • M.N. Avraamides et al.

    Multiple systems of spatial memory and action

    Cognitive Processes

    (2008)

  • A.D. Baddeley

    Working memory

    Science

    (1992)

  • A.D. Baddeley

    Exploring the central executive

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology

    (1996)

  • A.D. Baddeley

    Is working memory still working?

    European Psychologist

    (2002)

  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Random generation and the executive control of working memory

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1998)

  • D.H. Ballard et al.

    Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1997)

  • W.A. Bousfield

    The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged associates

    The Journal of General Psychology

    (1953)

  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    The international affective picture system (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    When you and I share perspectives: pronouns modulate perspective-taking during narrative comprehension

    Psychological Science

    (2009)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Up north and down south: implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2012)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    North is up(hill): route planning heuristics in real-world environments

    Memory & Cognition

    (2010)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    When goals constrain: eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2009)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    Learning procedures: the role of working memory in multimedia learning experiences

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2006)

  • T.T. Brunyé et al.

    The fabric of thought: priming tactile properties during reading influences direct tactile perception

    Cognitive Science

    (2012)

  • T.J. Bungum et al.

    The association of distraction and caution displayed by pedestrians at a lighted crosswalk

    Journal of Community Health

    (2005)

  • G.E. Burnett

    “Turn right at the traffic lights”: the requirements for landmarks in vehicle navigation systems

    The Journal of Navigation

    (2000)

  • G.E. Burnett et al.

    The effect of vehicle navigation systems on the formation of cognitive maps

  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: a critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)

  • L.A. Carlson-Radvansky et al.

    Inhibition accompanies reference-frame selection

    Psychological Science

    (1998)

  • B.L. Chalfonte et al.

    Feature memory and binding in young and older adults

    Memory & Cognition

    (1996)

  • M. Chen et al.

    Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus

    Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1999)

  • S.-A. Christianson

    Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: a critical review

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1992)

  • G.L. Clore et al.

    Affective feelings as feedback: some cognitive consequences

  • Y. Corson et al.

    Emotion and false memory: valence or arousal?

    Psychological Science

    (2007)

  • K.R. Coventry et al.

    Saying, seeing, and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions

    (2004)

  • Navigate DownView more references

    Cited by (6)

    • Cognitive strategies in the mental rotation task revealed by EEG spectral power

      2017, Brain and Cognition

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The classic mental rotation task (MRT; Shepard & Metzler, 1971) is commonly thought to measure mental rotation, a cognitive process involving covert simulation of motor rotation. Yet much research suggests that the MRT recruits both motor simulation and other analytic cognitive strategies that depend on visuospatial representation and visual working memory (WM). In the present study, we investigated cognitive strategies in the MRT using time-frequency analysis of EEG and independent component analysis. We scrutinized sensorimotor mu (µ) power reduction, associated with motor simulation, parietal alpha (pα) power reduction, associated with visuospatial representation, and frontal midline theta (fmθ) power enhancement, associated with WM maintenance and manipulation. µ power increased concomitant with increasing task difficulty, suggesting reduced use of motor simulation, while pα decreased and fmθ power increased, suggesting heightened use of visuospatial representation processing and WM, respectively. These findings suggest that MRT performance involves flexibly trading off between cognitive strategies, namely a motor simulation-based mental rotation strategy and WM-intensive analytic strategies based on task difficulty. Flexible cognitive strategy use may be a domain-general cognitive principle that underlies aptitude and spatial intelligence in a variety of cognitive domains. We close with discussion of the present study’s implications as well as future directions.

    • A Reflection on Noncognitive Factors Affecting Spatial Cognitive Testing: Examples From Nonmodel Species

      2022, Journal of Comparative Psychology

    • Exploring the Spatial Distribution Characteristics and Correlation Factors of Wayfinding Performance on City-Scale Road Networks Based on Massive Trajectory Data

      2021, Journal of Advanced Transportation

    • Influence of familiarity on the driving behavior, route risk, and route choice of older drivers

      2019, IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems

    • The path more travelled: Time pressure increases reliance on familiar route-based strategies during navigation

      2017, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    • Strategies for selecting routes through real-world environments: Relative topography, initial route straightness, and cardinal direction

      2015, PLoS ONE

    • Research article

      Perceptual Learning, Cognition, and Expertise

      Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Volume 58, 2013, pp. 117-165

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Recent research indicates that perceptual learning (PL)—experience-induced changes in the way perceivers extract information—plays a larger role in complex cognitive tasks, including abstract and symbolic domains, than has been understood in theory or implemented in instruction. Here, we describe the involvement of PL in complex cognitive tasks and why these connections, along with contemporary experimental and neuroscientific research in perception, challenge widely held accounts of the relationships among perception, cognition, and learning. We outline three revisions to common assumptions about these relations: 1) Perceptual mechanisms provide complex and abstract descriptions of reality; 2) Perceptual representations are often amodal, not limited to modality-specific sensory features; and 3) Perception is selective. These three properties enable relations between perception and cognition that are both synergistic and dynamic, and they make possible PL processes that adapt information extraction to optimize task performance. While PL is pervasive in natural learning and in expertise, it has largely been neglected in formal instruction. We describe an emerging PL technology that has already produced dramatic learning gains in a variety of academic and professional learning contexts, including mathematics, science, aviation, and medical learning.

    • Research article

      Point-to-point ligand–receptor interactions across the subunit interface modulate the induction and stabilization of conformational states of alpha7 nAChR by benzylidene anabaseines

      Biochemical Pharmacology, Volume 85, Issue 6, 2013, pp. 817-828

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The homomeric α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor is a well-studied therapeutic target, though its characteristically rapid desensitization complicates the development of drugs with specific agonist effects. Moreover, some experimental compounds such as GTS-21 (2,4diMeOBA), a derivative of the α7-selective partial agonist benzylidene anabaseine (BA), produce a prolonged residual desensitization (RD) in which the receptor remains non-activatable long after the drug has been removed from extracellular solution. In contrast, the desensitization caused by GTS-21's dihydroxy metabolite (2,4diOHBA) is relatively short-lived. RD is hypothetically due to stable binding of the ligand to the receptor in its desensitized state. We can attribute the reduction in RD to a single BA hydroxyl group on the 4′ benzylidene position. Computational prediction derived from homology modeling showed the serine36 (S36) residue of α7 as a reasonable candidate for point-to-point interaction between BA compounds and the receptor. Through evaluating the activity of BA and simple derivatives on wild-type and mutant α7 receptors, it was observed that the drug–receptor pairs which were capable of hydrogen bonding at residue 36 exhibited significantly less stable desensitization. Further experiments involving the type II positive allosteric modulator (PAM) PNU-120596 showed that the various BA compounds’ preference to induce either a PAM-sensitive (Ds) or PAM-insensitive (Di) desensitized state is concentration dependent and suggested that both states are destabilized by S36 H-bonding. These results indicate that the fine-tuning of agonists for specific interaction with S36 can facilitate the development of therapeutics with targeted effects on ion channel desensitization properties and conformational state stability.

    • Research article

      Space, Time, and Story

      Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Volume 58, 2013, pp. 47-76

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Life presents as a continuous multimodal barrage on all our senses. From that, we abstract events, discrete units characterized by completion of goals and peaks of action. Effective communication of sequences of events in explanations and narratives is similarly segmented, and linked globally by overall themes and locally by anaphora. Visuospatial explanations and narratives, notably diagrams, comics, and gestures, rely on congruity of mappings of elements and relations of ideas to space and marks in space. Just as we design visuospatial discourse, we design the world: Our design actions in space create diagrams in the world, patterns, piles, rows, one-to-one correspondences, and the like, that express abstractions, categories, hierarchies, dimensions, and more, a circular process termed spraction.

    • Research article

      Causation, Touch, and the Perception of Force

      Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Volume 58, 2013, pp. 167-202

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Much of the thinking on causation recognizes that it entails more than spatial–temporal contiguity or correlation, but it has been difficult to specify exactly what that extra component of thought is. In this paper, we argue that the representation of causal relations is based on the feeling of force as understood through the sense of touch. Grounding causation in people’s sense of touch allows us to address the long-standing challenges that have been raised against force-based approaches to causation. In support of our proposal, we review research on the perception of causation that provides support for a force-based view of causation. We also describe recent findings that establish a direct connection between people’s impressions of causation and their sense of touch. We conclude by showing how a force-based view can be extended to handle the problem of how abstract causal relations are represented and acquired.

    • Research article

      Individual Differences in Intelligence and Working Memory: A Review of Latent Variable Models

      Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Volume 58, 2013, pp. 233-270

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The purpose of the current review is to examine individual differences in intelligence and working memory capacity. The emphasis is on latent variable models and theoretical frameworks that connect interindividual differences in behavior with intraindividual psychological processes. Our review suggests that intelligence and working memory capacity are strongly correlated and that the shared variance is primarily due to the fluid reasoning component of intelligence and mechanisms of cognitive control in working memory. We conclude that research on intelligence and working memory is a rare successful example of the unification of experimental and differential psychology. Finally, we argue that general ability models of intelligence that posit a unitary source of variance are not consistent with contemporary research and should be fairly rejected.

    • Research article

      Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes

      Cognition, Volume 129, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 426-438

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Processes for perspective-taking can be differentiated on whether or not they require us to mentally rotate ourselves into the position of the other person (). Until now, only two perspective-taking tasks have been differentiated in this way, showing that judging whether something is to someone’s left or right does require mental rotation, but judging if someone can see something or not does not. These tasks differ firstly on whether the content of the perspective is visual or spatial and secondly on whether the type of the judgement is early-developing (level-1 type) or later-developing (level-2 type). Across two experiments, we tested which of these factors was likely to be most important by using four different perspective-taking tasks which crossed orthogonally the content of judgement (visual vs. spatial) and the type of judgement (level-1 type vs. level-2 type). We found that the level-2 type judgements, of how something looks to someone else and whether it is to their left or right, required egocentric mental rotation. On the other hand, level-1 type judgements, of whether something was in front of or behind someone and of whether someone could see something or not, did not involve mental rotation. We suggest from this that the initial processing strategies employed for perspective-taking are largely independent of whether judgements are visual or spatial in nature. Furthermore, early developing abilities have features that make mental rotation unnecessary.

      What is the cognitive process model?

      The cognitive process model framework is a conceptual framework meant for descriptive models of cognitive processes; it is applicable to models before and after model testing.

      Why is cognitive process important?

      We need cognition to help us understand information about the world around us and interact safely with our environment, as the sensory information we receive is vast and complicated: cognition is needed to distill all this information down to its essentials.

      Why are models important in cognitive psychology?

      Clinical psychologists use cognitive models to assess individual differences in cognitive processing between normal individuals and clinical patients (e.g., schizophrenics). Cognitive neuroscientists use cognitive models to under- stand the psychological function of different brain regions.

      What are the three main aspects of cognition?

      Cognition involves perception, attention, and memory. Cognitive theory seeks to understand human learning, socialization, and behavior by looking at the brain's internal cognitive processes.