In general, socioeconomic status is better than ethnicity at predicting what student outcome?

Which of the following shows attributes of Maslow's hierarchy in order from lowest to highest?

A. Physiological, safety, love and belongingness, esteem
B. Esteem, love and belongingness, safety, physiological
C. Safety, physiological, love and belongingness, esteem
D. Physiological, safety, self-actualization, esteem

ABSTRACT

The authors investigated how different dimensions of parental involvement similarly or differentially linked to various constructs of school motivation (academic self-efficacy in mathematics and English, intrinsic motivation toward mathematics and English, and engagement) across ethnic groups of Caucasian, African American, Asian American, and Hispanic students. The structural equation modeling analyses revealed the existence of ethnic differences by providing evidence that (a) parental advising and parent–school communication concerning benign school issues were positively related to Hispanic students’ intrinsic motivation toward English and academic self-efficacy in English, but negatively related to Asian American students’ mathematics intrinsic motivation and/or mathematics self-efficacy; and (b) parental participation in school functions sporadically affected the school motivational constructs for only Caucasian and African American students. Despite the ethnic differences, similar findings across ethnic groups were also noted. Generally, results showed that parental aspiration for children's education positively related to student school motivational constructs, whereas school–parent communication regarding student school problems negatively predicted student school motivational constructs across ethnic groups.

  • Access through your institution

Volume 97, July 2022, 102161

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2022.102161Get rights and content

For more than three decades, hundreds of studies on self-determination theory (SDT: Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan and Deci, 2000, Ryan and Deci, 2020) have been published that yielded important insights into student motivation, learning, and well-being (Howard et al., 2021; Stroet et al., 2013; Yu et al., 2018). A key finding of these studies is the positive influence of need-supportive teaching on students' academic achievement by facilitating intrinsic motivation. When teachers and contexts support students' basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness, students flourish. SDT asserts that the association of need-supportive teaching with students' positive learning outcomes is cross-culturally generalizable (Reeve et al., 2018; Ryan & Deci, 2000). There is extensive empirical evidence that need-supportive teaching promotes student motivation and various positive learning outcomes across different socio-cultural contexts (e.g., Chen et al., 2015; Reeve et al., 2018; Ryan & Deci, 2020; Stroet et al., 2013).

However, many SDT cross-cultural studies only examined SDT tenets in a single non-Western country (e.g., Jang et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2019) or compared groups of countries in terms of Western and Eastern cultures (e.g., Chirkov et al., 2003; Jiang et al., 2021; Nalipay et al., 2020). However, these studies usually operationalized culture in terms of country of origin or ethnicity which might be a somewhat simplistic operationalization of culture (e.g., Cohen & Varnum, 2016; King et al., 2018; King & McInerney, 2014). Schwartz (2006) proposed a cultural grouping based on value orientation. As of this current writing, there has been no research that tested SDT's cross-cultural generalizability using this lens. Furthermore existing research has mainly focused on autonomy support (e.g., Diseth & Samdal, 2014; Haerens et al., 2015; Kaplan, 2018; Patall et al., 2018). Relatively fewer studies have investigated teachers' support for other basic needs such as competence and relatedness (Stroet et al., 2013). Hence, there is a need for studies that examine the three need-supportive practices altogether with a more fine-grained cross-cultural testing.

The presence of large-scale international assessments such as the Programme for International Student Assessments (PISA) affords researchers the means to investigate SDT's generalizability across a broader group of countries from different sociocultural contexts (e.g., Jiang et al., 2021; Lee, 2014). The present study aimed to investigate the role of need-supportive teaching in students' motivation and achievement across a wide range of cultures. We drew on data from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), consisting of samples from 76 regions or societies. These regions were grouped into eight distinct cultural groups: Western Europe, Eastern Central Europe, Eastern Europe, Latin America, English speaking, Confucian, Southeast Asia, and Africa and the Middle East (Schwartz, 2006). We tested whether students' perceived need-supportive teaching would stimulate intrinsic motivation, which in turn would predict optimal achievement.

SDT presupposes that everyone has basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. The satisfaction of these needs is dependent on the socio-contextual environment. Autonomy means having a sense of volition or self-regulation in one's behavior or subjective experience. Competence relates to having the feeling of being able to meet an optimal challenge. Relatedness is having the sense of being connected to an individual or group (Ryan and Deci, 2000, Ryan and Deci, 2017). Supporting and satisfying these psychological needs facilitates intrinsic and autonomous motivation, leading to positive cognitive, affective, and behavioral outcomes (Reeve, 2012). On the other hand, thwarting these needs leads to controlled motivation, maladaptive behaviors, and even amotivation (Ryan & Deci, 2017).

In the educational context, the teacher is the students' proximal social context who can either support or thwart their students' needs (Ahn et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2016). Hence, the quality of the teacher's support influences the students' quality of motivation, which in turn affects the student's engagement and other learning outcomes (Jang et al., 2016; Reeve, 2016; Y. Wang et al., 2021). Researchers have specified key teacher practices that could demonstrate autonomy support, competence support or structure, and relatedness support or involvement (Ahn et al., 2019; Niemiec & Ryan, 2009; Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Autonomy support allows students to be more self-directed. It comes in the form of explaining the rationale or importance of learning materials or activities, providing meaningful choices, acknowledging students' emotions, avoiding controlling language, and nurturing inner motivational resources (Ahn et al., 2019; Niemiec & Ryan, 2009; Reeve, 2016). Competence support involves creating a structure that fosters a conducive learning environment where students can focus and develop themselves to meet learning challenges. Teachers create structure by setting clear goals and expectations, offering tasks with moderate difficulty to expand students' academic capability, and offering feedback to promote a sense of efficacy and success (Ahn et al., 2019; Niemiec & Ryan, 2009; Reeve, 2016). Relatedness support involves making students feel they belong and are connected through the teachers' involvement (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009). Teachers show involvement by communicating interest, appreciation, and concern for students (Ahn et al., 2019; Niemiec & Ryan, 2009).

Studies have shown that need-supportive teaching has beneficial effects on key student outcomes. Need-supportive teaching has been associated with higher resilience (Pitzer & Skinner, 2017), greater enjoyment, and lower fear of failure (Leptokaridou et al., 2016). Students become more engaged, learn more, and attain higher performance when taught in a need-supportive way (Jang et al., 2016; Reeve, 2013; Taylor et al., 2014; H. Wang et al., 2021). However, many of these studies have focused only on limited aspects of need-supportive teaching. For example, Jang et al. (2012) focused only on teachers' autonomy support and found it was associated with greater engagement among Korean students. Other researchers have focused on the effects of structure in supporting the need for competence (e.g., Eckes et al., 2018; Guay et al., 2017). Some other studies simultaneously examined autonomy and competence support on students' psychological outcomes (e.g., Hospel & Galand, 2016; Oga-Baldwin & Nakata, 2015; Olivier et al., 2020). A few studies also investigated the role of supporting relatedness and found its association with greater engagement and achievement (e.g., Furrer & Skinner, 2003; King, 2015). Taken altogether, these studies show that each of the dimensions of need-supportive teaching seems to be associated with optimal outcomes. However, research that simultaneously examines all three needs seem lacking, signifying a gap in the knowledge base (Stroet et al., 2013). The present study aims to address this gap by taking support for all three needs simultaneously.

Supporting the three basic psychological needs creates an environment that stimulates intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation refers to the natural propensity to do an action because it is enjoyable and does not depend on external contingencies (Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDT links intrinsic motivation directly with optimal learning outcomes, as supported by various studies (Dickhäuser et al., 2016; Ryan & Deci, 2020; Taylor et al., 2014; Vansteenkiste et al., 2009). For example, studies have shown that intrinsically motivated students have higher academic achievement in different learning domains such as reading (Froiland & Oros, 2014; Hebbecker et al., 2019) and mathematics (Froiland & Davison, 2016; Garon-Carrier et al., 2016). In recent years, studies using large-scale assessments such as PISA also provided evidence linking intrinsic motivation with student achievement (Lee, 2014).

Past research has also shown that intrinsic motivation mediates the influence of need-supportive teaching on engagement and academic achievement (de Naeghel et al., 2014; Fan & Williams, 2018; Hofferber et al., 2016; Yildirim, 2012). For example, Hofferber et al.'s (2016) quasi-experimental study found support for the indirect association of need-supportive teaching on students' engagement in a biology class via greater intrinsic motivation. Moreover, Fan and Williams's (2018) study found that the perception of teacher-student relationships, which closely approximates relatedness support or involvement in class, influenced students' academic achievement in math and reading through intrinsic motivation. These studies demonstrate that need-supportive teaching stimulates intrinsic motivation, and intrinsic motivation, in turn, leads to better academic achievement (see also Ryan et al., 2019; E. Skinner, 1990; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020).

A fundamental assertion within SDT is that the three basic psychological needs are crucial for all individuals regardless of cultural background (Ryan and Deci, 2000, Ryan and Deci, 2017, Ryan and Deci, 2020). However, some cultural scholars have argued that SDT's basic propositions, especially concerning autonomy, are Western-centric (Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; Iyengar & DeVoe, 2003; Markus et al., 1996; Markus & Kitayama, 2003; Miller et al., 2017). For example, Miller et al. (2017) argued that SDT's universal claim does not consider cultural nuances and variability. Furthermore Markus and Kitayama (2003) suggested that freedom of choice might be more relevant to Western cultures but less so for collectivist cultures. For Eastern cultures, restricting choice does not always lead to negative effects if done by a trusted authority or figure (e.g., Iyengar & Lepper, 2000). Hence, the debate on autonomy vs. choice revolves around whether it applies to all cultures. Most SDT theorists address the debate by zeroing on the misinterpretation of autonomy as choice or independence. More importantly, they also clarified the misinterpretation of universality as mere uniformity (e.g., Reeve et al., 2018).

Scholars argue that there are two extreme positions in cross-cultural studies (King & McInerney, 2014; Pintrich, 2003;Shweder & Sullivan, 1993 ; Zusho & Clayton, 2011). On the one hand, there are absolutists who assume that there are motivational values and processes that are essentially culture free. On the other hand, there are the relativists who argue that motivational constructs should be studied in the local context and that everything is relative. In between these polarities are the universalists who see some basic processes that are universal but their expression differ across cultures (Zusho & Clayton, 2011). In line with the universalist view, SDT theorists recognize that the basic psychological needs support and satisfaction have cross-cultural significance while expecting cultural and individual variations in how they are expressed or valued (Reeve et al., 2018; Ryan et al., 2021; Ryan & Deci, 2019). Some SDT theorists use Shweder and Sullivan's (1993) term “universality without uniformity” or “moderate universality” to delineate it from absolutism (e.g., Soenens et al., 2015; van Assche et al., 2018; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). We adopt a universalist stance in this research in line with SDT theorizing and findings from motivation science (King & McInerney, 2014; Pintrich, 2003; Soenens et al., 2015; van Assche et al., 2018; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020; Zusho & Clayton, 2011).

Evidence from various studies has provided empirical support for SDT's cross-cultural universality. For example, Chirkov et al.'s (2003) study demonstrated that autonomy is a valid construct among non-Western students. They also showed that autonomy support was positively associated with students' well-being in Eastern cultures (i.e., Turkey and South Korea). Nalipay et al.'s (2020) investigated the 2009 PISA participating countries and demonstrated that autonomy is essential both for Eastern and Western countries. Researchers have also shown that need support facilitates positive outcomes among collectivist societies. Oga-Baldwin and Nakata (2015) found that need-supportive teaching is associated with sustained engagement over time among Japanese students. In another study, Zhang et al. (2020) demonstrated that Chinese students become more intrinsically motivated to study when their teachers shift to a need-supportive style.

Several studies have also examined the core aspects of SDT in other Asian contexts, such as the Philippines (Haw et al., 2021; Mendoza et al., 2022), Indonesia (Maulana et al., 2016), Thailand (Kaur et al., 2015), and South Korea (e.g., Reeve et al., 2004), among others. These studies demonstrate broad support for SDT's core postulates in non-Western cultures. Congruent to the moderate universality (Shweder & Sullivan, 1993; Zusho & Clayton, 2011), SDT research have reported significant findings across cultures and their variations (Deci et al., 2001; Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2016; Lam et al., 2016; Marbell-Pierre et al., 2019).

However, many of the studies above only focused on individual countries representing a specific cultural context. Moreover, SDT comparative studies tend to use a less nuanced definition of culture, such as the binary opposition of East and West. Such comparison is limiting because sociocultural contexts overlap with but are not entirely synonymous with nationality, ethnicity, or race (Cohen & Varnum, 2016; King et al., 2018; King & McInerney, 2014). These methodological limitations also limit empirical testing of SDT's cross-cultural generalizability hypothesis. There is a need to sift the more fine-grained aspects of culture that could explain similarities or differences in students' motivational dynamics and outcomes. One way to do this is to test SDT's claims across a broader range of cultural groups.

Schwartz’s (2006) cultural value orientation theory proposed that cultures can be distinguished by the values they espouse. These value preferences are based on three polarities: embeddedness vs. autonomy, hierarchy vs. egalitarianism, and mastery vs. harmony. These polarities simultaneously pull a cultural dynamic and create the direction of their values. We elucidate these dimensions below:

The first polarity of embeddedness vs. mastery pertains to the nature of the relationship between the person and the group. In cultures that emphasize embeddedness, people are viewed as embedded in the larger collective, with meaning in life derived primarily through social relationships. On the other hand, in cultures that emphasize autonomy, people are considered autonomous bounded entities and are encouraged to find meaning in their uniqueness. The second polarity of hierarchy vs. egalitarianism focuses on the issue of how people must behave toward one another to preserve the social fabric. Hierarchical cultures emphasize the importance of ascribed roles and responsibilities, with people taking the unequal distribution of power for granted. Social power, authority, humility, and wealth are considered primary. Egalitarian cultures, on the other hand, recognize people as equals who share a common interest. Cooperation and concern for everyone's welfare are emphasized. The third polarity of mastery vs. harmony focuses on how people should manage their relations to the natural and social worlds. Mastery cultures encourage active self-assertion and values ambition, success, and competence. On the other hand, harmony cultures emphasize fitting into the world and trying to understand and appreciate what is. World peace, unity with nature, and environmental protection are highly valued.

Using these three dimensions, Schwartz, 2006, Schwartz, 2009 analyzed samples from 76 countries, plotted them according to their cultural value orientation, and generated eight cultural regions. These eight groups are Western Europe, Eastern Central Europe, Eastern Europe, Latin America, English speaking, Confucian, Southeast Asia, and Africa and the Middle East. Fig. 1 shows these groupings plotted using their polar preferences.

Schwartz (2006) argued that each group is culturally distinct in emphasizing or de-emphasizing a value orientation. For example, Western Europe's (e.g., France and Germany) high emphasis on autonomy characterizes the culture as individualistic. However, its heightened focus on egalitarianism and harmony goes beyond “conventional views of individualism” (p. 55). Confucian and South Asian cultures emphasize hierarchy, yet they differ by emphasizing mastery and embeddedness, respectively.

Schwartz’s value orientations show the multi-dimensionality of culture, which goes beyond country, ethnicity, geographical location. It puts a fine-grained understanding of culture that challenges the typical broad cultural differentiations (i.e., West as individualistic vs. East as collectivist). Testing SDT's theoretical assumptions across Schwartz’s eight cultural groups, therefore, has critical implications for a stronger test of self-determination theory’s cross-cultural validity. As of this current writing, we are not aware of prior empirical studies that have tested the relations among need-supportive teaching, intrinsic motivation, and achievement among all eight cultural groups.

Studies have consistently documented that gender and socioeconomic status affect student motivation and reading achievement (e.g., Vecchione et al., 2014). For example, several studies have found that girls do better than boys in school overall though there are subject-specific differences (Guo et al., 2015; King, 2016; Voyer and Voyer, 2014). In terms of SES, several studies have found that students from advantaged families do better in school than their less advantaged counterparts. Motivational differences have also been documented, with children from higher SES families exhibiting more adaptive motivational outcomes (Benner et al., 2016; King & Trinidad, 2021; Sirin, 2005). Hence, we added gender and SES as covariates to control their possible confounding effects and ensure a more robust test of our core hypotheses.

The existing evidence base highlight the importance of need-supportive teaching and intrinsic motivation on student achievement. Given SDT's assertion on moderate universality, we aimed at examining these core tenets against Schwartz’s (2006) cultural value orientation grouping (i.e., Western Europe, Eastern Central Europe, Eastern Europe, Latin America, English speaking, Confucian, Southeast Asia, and Africa and the Middle East). Specifically, we investigated the following hypotheses across the eight groups:

H1

: Need-supportive teaching will positively predict student achievement.

H2

: Intrinsic motivation will positively predict student achievement.

H3

: Need-supportive teaching will indirectly predict reading achievement via reading intrinsic motivation.

Fig. 2 below shows the hypothesized conceptual model of the study.

We conducted the study following ethical guidelines for human subjects and approved by the relevant human research ethics review board. We utilized the publicly available data from the 2018 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) organized by the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) in this study (OECD, 2021). PISA is a triennial program that assesses 15-year-old students across OECD's 79 countries and regions in the following literacy domains: reading,

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations by cultural value orientation grouping, respectively. Reading intrinsic motivation showed a strong positive and significant correlation with reading achievement across all groups. Results further showed that need-supportive teaching correlates positively with reading intrinsic motivation across all eight cultural groups with a relatively small effect size. Likewise, the correlation between need-supportive teaching and reading

We aimed to investigate whether the associations among need-supportive teaching, intrinsic motivation, and achievement would hold across cultures. We used Schwartz’s (2009) model to classify different regions into eight cultural groups. First, we hypothesized that need-supportive teaching would predict reading achievement (H1). Second, we posited that intrinsic motivation would predict achievement (H2). Lastly, we hypothesized that need-supportive teaching would predict intrinsic motivation,

PISA 2018 dataset afforded this study the benefit of simultaneously analyzing large representative samples from different countries and regions. However, we also had to contend with the apparent issues and limitations in conducting secondary analysis using data from large-scale assessments: (a) causal relationships in PISA, (b) measurement, and (c) its limited generalizability. Hence, our findings should be interpreted with caution.

First, PISA is a cross-sectional survey from which causal

The present study demonstrates the potential benefit of need-supportive teaching in fostering students' achievement in reading through intrinsic motivation. Despite some limitations, the models tested held across the majority of the sampled cultural groups and regions. The study’s test of SDT's core tenets found broad—though not unanimous—support for the idea that students across the globe could benefit from teachers' need-supportive practices.

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available at https://www.oecd.org/pisa/data/2018database/

Not applicable.

None.

The authors would like to thank the five anonymous reviewers for their constructive feedback and helpful suggestions that greatly contributed to the quality of this research.

  • et al.
  • M.J.N. Nalipay et al.
  • L. Ma et al.
  • W.C. Liu et al.
  • M.J.M. Leenknecht et al.
  • V. Kurdi et al.
  • R.B. King
  • R.B. King
  • H. Jang et al.
  • V. Hospel et al.

  • J.Y. Haw et al.
  • L. Haerens et al.
  • J. Guo et al.
  • G.E. Gignac et al.
  • J.M. Froiland et al.
  • A. Eckes et al.
  • O. Dickhäuser et al.
  • E.L. Deci et al.
  • A.B. Cohen et al.
  • I. Ahn et al.
  • I. Ahn et al.
  • J.C. Anderson et al.
  • C. Artelt
  • A.D. Benner et al.
  • K.A. Bollen
  • F.A. Bosco et al.
  • D.H. Caro et al.
  • J.R. Cheema
  • B. Chen et al.
  • B. Chen et al.
  • V. Chirkov et al.
  • A.T. Church et al.
  • E. Davidov et al.
  • J. de Naeghel et al.
  • E.L. Deci et al.
  • A. Diseth et al.
  • W. Fan et al.
  • J.M. Froiland et al.
  • D.C. Funder et al.
  • C. Furrer et al.
  • G. Garon-Carrier et al.
  • F. Guay et al.
  • J.E. Gustafsson
  • M.S. Hagger et al.
  • J. He et al.
  • K. Hebbecker et al.
  • N. Hofferber et al.
  • J.L. Howard et al.
  • L.T. Hu et al.
  • F.L. Huang
    • Early identification is important for improving reading outcomes for children at risk for reading difficulties, but current methods tend to perform sub-optimally in identifying true risk. One possible reason is that whereas most prediction models assume linear relationships among risk and reading outcome measures, qualitatively different patterns of association may exist among the measures at different skill levels. We implemented dynamic non-linear modeling to test this possibility in two distinct samples of children: 1) 225 pre-kindergarten and kindergarten with concurrent data and 2) 104 children with longitudinal (pre-)kindergarten and second-grade reading scores. Cusp catastrophe modeling was used to evaluate the moderating effects of rapid automatized naming (RAN) and phonological processing, two foundational pre-reading skills, on the concurrent and longitudinal relationship between letter identification and word reading. We further tested whether RAN and phonological processing have independent non-linear effects on the letter-word reading relationship above and beyond that of a single skill. Deficits in RAN and phonological processing beyond a critical level were associated with non-linear changes in the prediction of word reading from letter knowledge, both concurrently and longitudinally, fully supporting the cusp model over the competing models. These findings demonstrate the importance of implementing non-linear models for predicting risk for reading difficulties. There was no evidence for the interactive effects of RAN and phonological processing on reading. Instead, in accordance with the basic tenants of the double-deficit hypothesis, current results suggest that the constructs represent two salient but separable causes of reading impairment, even at the earliest stages of reading ability. These findings suggest that models predicting which at-risk children will develop poor reading must diverge from assumptions of relationships observed in typical reading.

    • Self-regulated learning theory acknowledges the importance of an individual's metacognitive monitoring and group-level regulation for learning achievement in collaborative learning. However, very few studies have empirically investigated the interplay of these factors. This study aimed to investigate how groups' metacognitive interactions, group-level regulation, and individuals' metacognitive monitoring accuracy together predict students' learning achievement. Thirty groups of secondary school students (n = 94) attended a five-week physics course involving four 90-min lessons. Each lesson included a collaborative learning session in which the students solved physics problems in groups of three. After each session, the students filled out a quiz that evaluated their learning of the core concepts related to each lesson. The students' collaborative learning was video-recorded using 360-degree cameras. First, the groups' metacognitive interaction, co-regulation (CoRL), and socially shared regulation (SSRL) during collaborative learning were coded from the video data. Second, the students' metacognitive monitoring accuracy was determined in relation to quizzes presented at the end of each collaborative session. Each student's learning achievement was assessed at the end of the course with an individual written exam. Bayesian multilevel models were used to analyze the nested data. The results showed that the frequency of metacognitive interaction was positively related to learning achievement. However, the relation between CoRL and learning achievement was moderated by monitoring accuracy and metacognitive interaction. The relation between CoRL and achievement was positive when coupled with a high frequency of metacognitive interactions but negative under a low frequency of metacognitive interactions. In addition, in line with theory, the students with lower monitoring accuracy achieved higher learning when CoRL was frequent in a group. Due to the low occurrence of SSRL, its relation with learning achievement could not be analyzed. No reliable evidence for the relation between monitoring accuracy and learning achievement was found. The results highlighted the complex interplay of individual-and group-level factors related to collaborative learning outcomes.

    • In achievement motive theory, need for achievement is conceptualized as an explanatory variable that triggers certain experiences and behaviors spontaneously. Experience sampling should therefore be an ideal approach for capturing such motive-specific affective contingencies. However, given that not all students seem to be aware of their underlying motives, the link between self-reported need for achievement and daily experiences may depend on their mindfulness level. In a sample of university students (N = 107), self-reported fear of failure predicted momentary negative activation across activity contexts in everyday life. In addition, hope of success predicted positive activation in more mindful students and in nonroutine situations (e.g., studying, working, or leisure time). Together, these findings are a first step toward illuminating the phenomenological and excitatory nature of need for achievement in students' everyday lives and illustrate the necessity of noticing motive-specific cues in order to integrate them into the explicit motivational self-concept.

    • This study examined the effects of a utility value (UV) intervention on students' situational interest and boredom in science using hierarchical linear growth modeling. Data were collected in a diverse sample of 339 students in 13 seventh and ninth grade science classrooms using surveys and end-of-class reports collected on 11 occasions. Results showed no UV intervention effects on students' trajectories of situational boredom. Contrary to expectations, both seventh and ninth graders in classrooms with the UV intervention showed small but significant declines in situational interest over time relative to a writing comparison group. Intervention effects were not moderated by success expectancies. Explanations for these unexpected findings, as well as implications and future directions, are discussed.

    • Intervention research in education is sometimes criticized for the use of experimenter developed assessments, especially when these are over aligned with treatment. At the same time, intervention researchers sometimes prefer locally developed assessments because they appear to be more sensitive to treatment effects even when the test is not subject to the criticism of over alignment. This paper examines the question of test sensitivity to treatment effects for experimenter developed and standardized tests for the specific case of reading in grade 8. We examine similarities and differences between a specific experimenter developed test and widely used standardized reading assessment. Analyses show these particular tests to be quite comparable. The paper concludes with an examination of test sensitivity by simulating treatment effects of different magnitudes. These analyses highlight some potential limitations of the standardized test for detecting small to moderate effects depending on the ability range of the students participating in intervention. The implications for intervention research and identification of students under response to intervention are discussed.

    • There are ongoing efforts to understand female adolescents' motivation in math, as part of the science, technology, engineering, and math fields. The current study aimed to examine the temporal relations among expectancies, task values (intrinsic and utility value), and perceived costs (emotional and effort cost) in math to understand the underlying motivational processes for math-related career intention and achievement among middle school girls. To this end, we collected responses from 638 Korean middle school girls three separate times within a year. The autoregressive cross-lagged results revealed that there was no reciprocal relationship between expectancies and task values, but there appeared to be unidirectional positive prediction paths from utility value to self-efficacy. Regarding the costs, there were reciprocal relations between intrinsic value and emotional cost, which negatively affected each other. Consequently, utility value indirectly enhanced girls' math achievement by fostering self-efficacy, whereas emotional cost indirectly hindered their career intentions in math by dampening their intrinsic value. These findings suggest that it is important not only to increase task values, but also to manage costs to help adolescent girls adhere to their math-related career goals.

    View full text

    What have researchers concluded about the skills and work habits that children have when they enter kindergarten?

    What have researchers concluded about the skills and work habits that children have when they enter kindergarten? They are among the best predictors of academic motivation and performance throughout the elementary and secondary school years.

    Which of the following is the most likely characteristic of students who are perfectionists?

    Which of the following is the most likely characteristic of students who are perfectionists? They are prone to low self-esteem.

    What is the primary emphasis of efficacy training?

    Efficacy training : Efficacy training seeks to improve students' efficacy perceptions or belief in their ability to master a given task. The primary emphasis of efficacy training is Improving students' academic confidence.

    Which of the following is one of the main goals of effective classroom management?

    Effective classroom management has two main goals: to help students spend more time on learning and less time on non-goal-directed activity, and to prevent students from developing academic and emotional problems.